A Comparison of Laboratory-Reared Stock and Captured Fruit Flies (Drosophila melanogaster) using Upward Movement, Phototaxic, and Starvation Assays Reveals Significant Behavioral Differences
نویسندگان
چکیده
Fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) are model research organisms and are frequently reared in research institutions. Research specimens domesticated in a laboratory-reared setting may have different behavioral phenotypes as compared to their wild counterpart. Additionally, it has been determined that the absence of “key” stimuli in the physical environment of captive animals may result in altered behavioral patterns1. The purpose of this investigation was to test for differences in the behavioral phenotype of outdoor captured fruit flies as compared to laboratory-reared fruit flies with the use of three tests: an Upward Movement Assay, a Starvation Assay, and a Phototaxic Assay. Results from the Upward Movement Assay demonstrated statistically significant differences in the vertical moving speed of laboratory-reared flies and outdoor-caught flies. Results from the Phototaxic Assay revealed outdoor captured fruit flies exhibited a natural phototaxic behavior while laboratory-reared flies exhibited an inverse phototaxic behavior. The Starvation Assay proved that flies recently descended from outdoor caught fruit flies were able to withstand starvation twice as long as laboratory-reared flies. These results indicate a strong behavioral difference between flies that are descended from laboratory stock and flies that are caught from the outdoors. Research regarding the differences in domesticated organisms is an imperative topic for study because domestication and genetic drift have the potential to alter the behavioral phenotype. Changes in the behavioral phenotype may jeopardize the results of research experiments. Thus it is crucial to have a thorough comprehension of the behavior of outdoor caught fruit flies as compared to the behavior of their wild counterpart. It is suggested scientists change their fruit fly stock every few hundred generations in an effort to protect the natural gene pools of organisms which are bred in captivity for extended periods of time. Introduction Domestication is the evolutionary genetic change arising from the transition of a population from nature to deliberate human cultivation2. Animals have been domesticated both unconsciously and methodically since the end of the Pleistocene Era. Due to cultivation and routine human interactions with species, selection pressures are created and the effected species is forced to adapt to a new environment. The transition of a free-living culture to captive status is often accompanied by changes in availability A Comparison of Laboratory-Reared Stock and Captured Fruit Flies (Drosophila melanogaster) using Upward Movement, Phototaxic, and Starvation Assays Reveals Significant Behavioral Differences Trinity Russell1* and Richard Kurtz2 Student1, Teacher2: Commack High School, 1 Scholar Lane, Commack, NY 11725 *Corresponding author: [email protected] IN SCHOOL ARTICLE and/or accessibility of shelter, space, food, water, predation, and the social environment1. It is implied that the phenotype of the domesticated species will differ from its counterpart once it has undergone domestication. As natural selection has the ability to change the frequency of traits within a population, it has been discovered that the brain of an organism has the tendency to shrink when bred in captivity for extended periods of time3. Two approaches have been developed to gain a better understanding of the domestication process: a comparison of wild and domestic stocks (of a species), and the study of wild and domestic hybrids4. This experiment utilized the first approach, a comparison of the wild and domestic stocks of a species, to understanding the domestication process. The wild stock (outdoor captured fruit flies) is used as a representative and ancestor of the domesticated population (laboratory-reared fruit flies). A comparative approach between specific populations of wild versus domesticated animals at a single point in time is applied to this study. This experiment and other similar research tests suggest altered behaviors are a result of altered genes. Specific alleles have a relatively large impact on the development of behavioral characteristics specific to domesticated animals. For example, a study was conducted to identify the genetic variation between cultivated rice and its wild progenitor. This study assessed the genetic basis of the changes associated with the process of rice domestication. A total of 19 traits related to domestication in cultivated rice were discovered5. Another study demonstrated that laboratory-reared flies and wild fruit flies exhibit differences in an ovipositor choice test6. In the choice tests, using white and black artificial ovipositor domes, the wild flies’ selected black domes almost exclusively, but the laboratory-reared flies failed to display any preference6. Additionally, a study compared the behavior of wild and domestic stocks of Brook Trout. Results proved the domesticated group was much more vulnerable to trap-netting than the wild groups. After a week of trapping fish, 84% of the captured fish were from the domesticated group7. Drosophila’s favorable characteristics make it an ideal research specimen. They require minimal care, space, and equipment. In addition, fruit flies are easily cultivated in the laboratory; they have a high fecundity, and a short generation time. Thus, they are highly susceptible to genetic drift and domestication. Drosophila is presently one of the most commonly used model organisms in biological research. They serve as a genetic model for numerous diseases. They are also used to study aging, oxidative stress, immunity, diabetes, cancer, obesity and drug abuse8. Laboratory-reared fruit flies are essential in biological research and it is crucial to have a thorough understanding of their Trinity Russell and Richard Kurtz Page 2 of 5 Materials and Methods Outdoor captured flies were caught from locations on Long Island, New York: Commack, East Northport, and Mount Sinai. Strain Oregon R laboratory reared fruit flies were obtained from Carolina Biological. These flies are known to have descended from flies cultivated in the laboratory for 54 years. Carolina Biological’s fruit fly culture has remained unchanged without the addition of fruit flies from external sources. Both outdoor captured and laboratory-reared fruit flies were cultivated in the school laboratory for no more than 20 generations. Fruit flies used for this experiment were descendants of either outdoor captured or laboratory-reared locations, and they were all cultivated at the same ambient conditions. The fruit fly life cycle consist of four stages: egg, larva, pupa and adult. At a standard classroom temperature of 21oC, the fruit fly life cycle lasts two weeks. Flies were re-cultivated every two weeks. The Upward Movement Assay was used to determine the flies’ upward velocity toward a light source (figure 1). This assay consisted of a laboratory clamp, a 35cm glass tube with a 0.5 cm diameter, a bright light, a fly aspirator, syringe, and a stopwatch. This experiment was conducted in a dark room. The glass tube was held vertically by the laboratory clamp and the bright light was attached to the top of the glass tube. Flies were individually aspirated and syringed into the glass tube. The 25cm flight time was recorded. Data was averaged and recorded as time per centimeter. Each fly location consisted of 5 blocks and every block had 10 trials. In total, the Upward Movement Assay was comprised of 200 trials (Supplementary tables 1-4). The Phototaxic Assay was used to determine the strength of the flies’ phototaxic behavior. This assay consisted of Plexiglas, a syringe, a fruit fly aspirator, a bright light, and two flight arenas (light and dark). Plexiglas was cut into 3 fragments and glued together to create narrow corridors (2mm diameter) for the flies to travel (figure 2). One narrow canal branched off into two canals. At the end of each canal was either a light or dark flight arena. The light flight arena was illuminated with an incandescent light and the dark flight arena was fully covered in black construction paper. The remainder of the apparatus was enclosed in red translucent paper. Flies are unable to distinguish red light and this prevented the flies from getting distracted by excess light. Fruit flies were injected into the device and they showed an attraction for either the light or dark flight arena. The data from this assay was recorded in 5 blocks: each block contained 5 trials totaling 100 trials (Supplementary tables 5-8). The Starvation Assay was used to test the flies’ ability to withstand starvation. This assay consisted of vials with moist cotton. Ten flies from each location were anesthetized and placed into vials with moist cotton. At the end of each day the number of flies alive were recorded. Each fly location had 5 blocks of 10 flies, totaling 200 trials (Supplementary tables 9-12). behavior compared to the behavior of a fly originating from a natural environment. Laboratory-reared fruit flies which are domesticated have the ability to jeopardize the validity of significant experimental results. Although this is known, domestication of fruit flies within a laboratory is hardly taken into consideration. The laboratory-reared fruit flies were reared in the laboratory for approximately 1,500 generations which made them an ideal specimen for a study of domestication. Outdoor captured and laboratory reared fruit flies were both genetically variable. However, low genetic variation due to a consistent environment, laboratory conditions such as small culture vials, and genetic drift, accounts for predictably less genetic variation among the laboratory reared population9. Neither of the fly populations were isogenic. Descendants of flies caught outdoors (wild) and descendants of laboratory-reared fruit flies (domesticated) were tested for differences in behavioral phenotypes with the use of three assays: an Upward Movement Assay, a Phototaxic Assay, and a Starvation Assay. The Upward Movement Assay was used to test the flies’ vertical movement. The Phototaxic Assay was used to test fruit flies locomotive movement in response to the stimulus of light (phototaxic behavior). The Starvation Assay was constructed to test the flies’ abilities to withstand starvation. Figure 1. Upward Movement Assay Design. The glass tube was held vertically by the laboratory clamp and the bright light was attached to the top of the glass tube. Figure 2. Phototaxic Assay Design. Plexiglas was glued together to create a narrow corridor for the flies to travel. One narrow canal diverged to either a light or dark flight arena. The light flight arena was illuminated with a bright light and the dark flight arena was covered in black paper. The apparatus was enclosed in red translucent paper. Figure 3. Upward Movement Assay. Flies from outdoor locations (Mount Sinai, East Northport, and Commack) traveled 1cm in an average of approximately 1 second. Flies from Carolina Biological (lab-reared) traveled 1cm in an average of 6.5 seconds. (Error bars = Standard error) Trinity Russell and Richard Kurtz Page 3 of 5 Results Laboratory-reared fruit flies exhibit behavior which was different from its wild counterpart. They exhibited a different locomotive behavior, a stronger phototaxic behavior, and they were able to survive longer when starved. Results from the Upward Movement Assay demonstrate a statistical difference in vertical flight of lab reared and wild caught flies. Flies which were reared in laboratories took approximately six times longer to travel the same distance as flies which were captured from the outdoors. In addition, the behaviors of laboratoryreared flies were observed to be sporadic and inconsistent (figure 3). Results were assessed with an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA Microsoft Excel). A single factor 1Way Analysis of Variance Test (ANOVA) was applied to the data. The upward movement probability was 1.12E-06 indicating that there was a statistical difference in the vertical flight speed of the flies (due mostly to the difference in lab-reared flies). Results from the Phototaxic Assay demonstrate a statistical difference in phototaxic behavior of lab reared and wild caught flies (p = 0.004). Flies which were caught from the outdoors all exhibited a natural phototaxic behavior. Flies which were reared in laboratories exhibited an inverse phototaxic behavior (figure 4). A non-parametric test was applied to the ordinal data. The chi square probability was 0.004. This means that there is evidence to support the alternative hypothesis that the behavior/movement is dependent on the origin of the fly. The probability of accepting the alternative hypothesis when the null hypothesis should have been accepted is very small. Results from the Starvation Assay show that fruit flies which are reared in laboratories survived a maximum of three days. Fruit flies which are captured from the outdoors survived a maximum of four to five days. Fruit flies which were reared in laboratories died more rapidly than flies which were captured from the outdoors (figure 5). Figure 4. Phototaxic Assay Data. Wild caught flies were mostly attracted to the light flight arena and laboratory reared flies were mostly attracted to the dark flight arena. Wild caught fruit flies exhibited a natural phototaxic behavior. Laboratory reared flies (Carolina biological) exhibited an inverse phototaxic behavior. (Error bars = Standard Error) Figure 5. Starvation Assay Data. Flies from outdoor captured locations (Mount Sinai, East Northport, and Commack) survived a maximum of four to five days when starved. Flies from Carolina Biological were all dead by the third day of starvation. (Error bars = Standard Error) Trinity Russell and Richard KurtzPage 4 of 6 References1.Price, E. O. (1999). Behavioral development in animalsundergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behavior Science. Volume65, Issue 3, 245-271. 2. Rose, M. R. (2006). Evolutionary domestication in Drosophilasubobscura. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. Volume 20, Issue 2,758-766. 3. The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, Edition1, John Murray, 1-102. Darwin, Charles, (1859: Volume 1). 4. Animal Domestication and Behavior, First edition, CABIPublishing. 1-63. Price, E. (2002, Volume 1). 5. Xiong, L. (1998) Identification of genetic factors controllingdomestication-related traits of rice using an F2 population ofa cross between Oryza satlva and O. ruflpogon . Theoretical andApplied Genetics. Volume 98, Issue 2, 243-251. 6. Greany, P. D. (1979). Oviposition behavior of laboratory-rearedand wild caribbean fruit flies (Anastrepha suspensa: Diptera:Tephritidae): II. selected physical influences. EntomologiaExperimentalis Et Applicata, Volume 26, Issue 3, 239-244. 7. Flick, W. (1962). Problems in Sampling Wild and DomesticStocks of Brook Trout (Salvelinus fontinalis). Transactions of theAmerican Fisheries society. Volume 91, Issue 2, 140-144. 8. Drosophila melanogaster: The Fruit Fly, Edition 1, Taylor &Francis, 157-162. Sang, J. H. (2001, Volume 1). 9. Moreno, Pamela, Aracena, Jimena. (2010) DifferentialStarvation Survival of Field Collected and Laboratory Wild TypeFruit Flies (Drosophila melanogaster). Paper presented at the annualmeeting of the Oklahoma Research Day, Cameron University, Lawton,OK. http://www.allacademic.com/meta/p484414_index.html. 10. Nevo, E. (2001) Evolution of Genome-Phenome Diversityunder Enviornmental Stress. Proceedings of the National Academy ofSciences of the United States of America. Volume 98, Issue 11, 6233-6240. 11. Ackermann, M. (2000, March). Experimental evolution ofaging, growth, and reproduction in fruit flies. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Science of the United States of America. Volume97, Issue 7, 3309-3313.DiscussionHumans can play a role as a buffer between an animal and itsenvironment. Fruit flies that are reared in laboratories havereduced sensitivity to their environment2. It is speculated that thelaboratory-reared fruit flies’ impaired vertical movement may becaused by a lack of predation. Laboratory-reared fruit flies mayhave not honed mechanisms for escaping predators. Without theneed to hone flight skills, laboratory-reared fruit flies may havedeveloped flight mechanisms that are different from their wildcounterpart. Fruit flies which are reared in laboratories may notbe exposed to a natural 12 hour light 12 hour dark photoperiod.It is speculated that an inconsistent photoperiod may cause a labreared fruit fly to develop an inverse attraction to light. It hasalso been discovered that wild caught flies had evolved bettermechanisms for storing sugars and fats as compared to laboratory-reared flies10. Higher extrinsic adult mortality rates leads to anearly reproductive effort, eclosion at an earlier age and a smallersized fruit fly11. Therefore, the inability to properly store fats andsugars, ultimately affect laboratory-reared offspring. A recentstudy which tests starvation in Oregon R fruit flies suggests thereis a correlation between the sex of a fruit fly and its ability towithstand starvation. Results suggest that wild female fruit fliessurvive longer than wild male flies. In addition, wild female fruitflies have been discovered to survive longer than both male andfemale laboratory reared flies. The sex of a fruit fly is a possiblesource of variability within the results of the three behavioralassays9.Laboratory reared, and outdoor captured fruit flies wereboth genetically variable. The impact of genetic diversityand its effect on the results is unknown due to the inability todetermine the level of variability for each fly location. Whileit may be expected for the wild population to display a greaterbehavioral variance, compared to the domesticated stock, theirbehavior was surprisingly similar. The results of the three assaysdepicted the laboratory reared population to have a greatervariation in behavior. The results allude to the assumption thatlaboratory reared fruit flies have a greater level of variability. Acomparative study of genetic variation between outdoor capturedand laboratory reared fruit flies would be a fruitful avenue foradditional research. Furthermore, domestication may possiblybe a result of chromosomal and clustered blocks of genes. Thisclustering of genes may provide explanations for the geneticbasis of domestication5.It has been shown that domestication has an importanteffect on the development of the domestic phenotype. Scientistswho use fruit flies for experimental purposes are transferringfree living insects to captive status. This domestic phenotyperesults in genetic drift, artificial selection, and relaxed selection1.Many organisms are domesticated in laboratory-reared settingsand these organisms may have a different behavioral phenotypefrom their wild counterpart. Changes in behavioral phenotypecan alter the outcome of experiments and invalidate scientificresults. Scientists frequently use laboratory-reared fruit flies asexperimental specimens and the effects domestication has on thebehavior of a fly should be considered with respect to the results.It is suggested scientists change their fruit fly stock every fewhundred generations. “Natural” gene pools should be protectedwhen breeding animals in captivity for extended periods of time.In the future, additional outdoor captured fruit flies fromLong Island will be compared to laboratory-reared fruit fliesfrom Long Island. Flies from various states and various parts ofthe world will be tested to further validate the results. A test willbe conducted to determine the rate of evolution for a fruit fly.With this data, scientists will be able to create a more precise timeperiod for changing and restoring their fruit fly stock. A study willalso be conducted to determine the specific genes responsible forthe domestication of a fruit fly. Trinity Russell and Richard KurtzPage 5 of 6 Table 1. Mount Sinai Upward Movement Data(wild caught, time in sec. to travel 1 cm).Table 2. East Northport Upward Movement Data(wild caught, time in sec. to travel 1 cm). Table 3. Commack Upward Movement Data(wild caught, time in sec. to travel 1 cm).Table 4. Carolina Biological Upward Movement Data(laboratoryreared, time in sec. to travel 1 cm). Table 5. Mount Sinai Phototaxic Data(wild caught, Light versus Dark).Table 6. East Northport Phototaxic Data(wild caught, Light versus Dark).Supplementary Data Trinity Russell and Richard KurtzPage 6 of 6 Table 7. Commack Phototaxic Data(wild caught, Light versus Dark).Table 8. Laboratory-reared Phototaxic Data(wild caught, Light versus Dark). Table 10. East Northport Starvation Data(wild caught, number of flies alive at the end of each day). Table 11. Commack Starvation Data(wild caught, number of flies alive at the end of each day).Table 12. Mount Sinai Starvation Data(laboratory-reared, number of flies alive at the end of eachday).Table 9. Mount Sinai Starvation Data(wild caught, number of flies alive at the end of each day).
منابع مشابه
Academic Advisory Board
Fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) are model research organisms and are frequently reared in research institutions. Research specimens domesticated in a laboratory-reared setting may have different behavioral phenotypes as compared to their wild counterpart. Additionally, it has been determined that the absence of “key” stimuli in the physical environment of captive animals may result in alt...
متن کاملAconitase and Developmental EndPointsasEarly IndicatorsofCellularToxicity Induced by Xenobiotics in Drosophila Melanogaster
Background: In this study, the toxicity of the different xenobiotics was tested on the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster model system. Methods: Fly larvae were raised on food supplemented with xenobioticsat different concentrations (sodium nitroprusside (0.1-1.5 mM), S-nitrosoglutathione (0.5-4 mM), and potassium ferrocyanide (1 mM)). Emergence of flies, food intake by larvae, and pupation h...
متن کاملConcentration dependent effect of morphine, aspirin, capsaicin and chili pepper hydro alcoholic extract on thermal and chemical pain model in fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster)
Introduction: Pain research using animal models is related to ethical concerns, so invertebrates and insects have been recommended by researchers. In the present study, the nociceptive and antinociceptive effects of capsaicin, aspirin, morphine and chili extract were examined using fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster) as an alternative for rodent pain model. Methods: Stage 3 of larvae and ad...
متن کاملNutrient-Dependent Impact of Microbes on Drosophila suzukii Development
Drosophila suzukii Matsumura is an invasive species of vinegar fly that has become a prominent pest of berries and other soft-skinned fruits. Unlike most other Drosophila species, female D. suzukii flies lay their eggs in ripening and ripe fruits and larvae develop within the fruit. To understand how D. suzukii larvae utilize ripe and ripening fruits, which usually have low levels of protein, w...
متن کاملNonrandom mating in Drosophila melanogaster laboratory populations derived from closely adjacent ecologically contrasting slopes at "Evolution Canyon".
Ecological differentiation of natural populations of Drosophila melanogaster, Drosophila simulans, and another drosophilid, Zaprionus tuberculatus, in "Evolution Canyon," Mount Carmel, Israel, is well established. The fitness complex of D. melanogaster includes oviposition temperature preferences, tolerance to high temperature, drought stress and starvation, and different longevity patterns. Th...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2012